How could you confuse a rubber hand for your own hand?


Why the Trick Works

There is nothing simple about the brain, or about the perception of self and other. In this case, though, at least one thing is clear: The premotor cortex is directing the misperception.

The premotor cortex, located in the motor cortex of the frontal lobe, plays a central role in body ownership. It guides the recognition that this arm is mine, the arm intertwined with mine is his, and the hammer I'm holding is an external object.

The rubber hand illusion has provided evidence of how the premotor cortex uses data to determine ownership. It apparently relies on multiple channels of sensory input, and it weights those channels as more or less reliable in making its decision.

The study plays with the senses of vision and touch: Visually, the subject can't see her own hand, but she can see the rubber hand, which is about where her hand would be if it were there; the fake hand looks a lot like hers; and it's being stroked. Tactilely, she feels her own, hidden hand being stroked, and it feels just like the stroking she can see.

The premotor cortex takes this in. (Positional input plays a role here, too, which is why the hand needs to be placed in a realistic location; the subject needs to perceive it as a viable stand-in for her own hand.) It integrates the visual data and the tactile data, which don't quite match up, and attempts to make sense of it all.

When faced with discrepancies between visual and tactile input, the brain tends to place higher value on the visual [source: Kruglinski]. The result in the rubber hand illusion goes something like this: I see a hand that looks a bit like mine, and I see someone stroking it. I feel my own hand being stroked. The hand I see being stroked must be my hand. That the subject has no tactile sense of the rubber hand is, in effect, tossed out of the equation.

Beyond explaining the brainwork behind a really cool party trick, this insight into the brain's integration and management of multiple, possibly contradictory sensory inputs might open the door to a greater understanding of a wide range of diseases with symptoms of body- and self-related distortions, including schizophrenia, phantom-limb syndrome, body dysmorphic disorder, anorexia and, of course, stroke [sources: Science Daily, Ravilious]. It could eventually tell us precisely why a particular stroke victim may no longer recognize her right arm as her own -- and maybe even how to put her perceived body back together.

For more information on body ownership, distortions of self, and a whole bunch of related illusions, check out the links below.

Author's Note: How could you confuse a rubber hand for your own hand?

The brain and its integration of sensory stimuli is an enormous topic, and I tried to limit this article's scope to the specifics of the rubber-hand illusion in the hopes of making it manageable for readers of all backgrounds (and avoid producing a tome). Note, however, that while the premotor cortex is at the center of the rubber hand illusion, as it appears to be in control of body ownership, the parietal cortex is also involved in sensory interpretation, and it appears to control body image (shape and size). In my brief mention of disease processes that may be elucidated by findings in studies like the rubber hand illusion, the parietal lobe plays a central role in at least two: anorexia and body dysmorphic disorder. There is a very cool illusion that sheds light on body-image disruptions, which you can read about here in PLOS Biology.

Related Articles

Sources

  • "Body illusions: Rubber hand illusion." New Scientist. March 18, 2009. (Oct. 8, 2012) http://www.newscientist.com/article/dn16809-body-illusions-rubber-hand-illusion.html
  • Dijkerman, HC. "It's personal! Visuotactile predictive mechanisms of peripersonal space." Abstract. Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences. June 2011. (Oct. 8, 2012) http://www.narcis.nl/research/RecordID/OND1342509/Language/en
  • Keim, Brandon. "Rubber Hand Trick Reveals Brain-Body Link." Wired. Aug. 25, 2008. (Oct. 8, 2012) http://www.wired.com/wiredscience/2008/08/rubber-hand-tri/
  • Kruglinski, Susan. "The Man Who Mistook a Rubber Hand for His Own." Discovery Magazine. Jan. 2, 2005. (Oct. 8, 2012) http://discovermagazine.com/2005/jan/man-mistook-rubber-hand
  • Longo, Matthew, et al. "Linking Pain and the Body: Neural Correlates of Visually Induced Analgesia." The Journal of Neuroscience, February 22, 2012 • 32(8):2601 Internet: http://www.jneurosci.org/content/32/8/2601.full.pdf (Oct. 10, 2012)
  • Ravilious, Kate. "How the Brain Builds Its Image of the Body." The Guardian. Nov. 28, 2005. (Oct. 9, 2012) http://www.guardian.co.uk/science/2005/nov/29/neuroscience.highereducation
  • Salisbury, David. "Putting the mind back into the body of schizophrenia." Vanderbilt University News. Oct. 31, 2011. (Oct. 12, 2012) http://news.vanderbilt.edu/2011/10/body-mind-schizophrenia/
  • "Scientists Produce Illusion of Body Swapping." Science Daily. Dec. 2, 2008. (Oct. 8, 2012) http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2008/12/081202115148.htm
  • "Tactile illusions: Seven ways to fool your sense of touch." New Scientist. (Oct. 8, 2012) http://www.newscientist.com/special/tactile-illusions
  • Thakkar, Katharine N, et al. (2011) "Disturbances in Body Ownership in Schizophrenia: Evidence from the Rubber Hand Illusion and Case Study of a Spontaneous Out-of-Body Experience." PLoS ONE 6(10): http://www.plosone.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0027089
  • "That's Not My Hand! How The Brain Can Be Fooled Into Feeling A Fake Limb." Science Daily. July 5, 2004. (Oct. 8, 2012) http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2004/07/040702093052.htm

More to Explore